PROXEMICS
. . . every cubic inch of space is a
miracle. --Walt Whitman (Leaves of Grass, "Miracles")
The
desire for personal mobility seems to be unstoppable--it is, perhaps, the
Irresistible Force. --Charles Lave (1992)
Spatial signs, signals
and cues. According to its founder, Edward T. Hall, proxemics is the
study of humankind's "perception and use of space" (Hall 1968:83).
Usage: Like facial expressions, gestures, and postures, space "speaks." The prime directive of proxemic space is that we may not come and go everywhere as we please. There are cultural rules and biological boundaries--explicit as well as implicit and subtle limits to observe--everywhere.
Body space I. Scientific research on how we communicate in private and public spaces began with studies of animal behavior (ethology) and territoriality in the 19th and early 20th centuries. In 1959, the anthropologist Edward Hall popularized spatial research on human beings--calling it proxemics--in his classic book, The Silent Language.
Body space II. Hall identified four bodily distances--intimate
(0 to 18 inches), personal-casual (1.5 to 4 feet),
social-consultive (4 to 10 feet), and public (10 feet and
beyond)--as key points in human spacing behavior. Hall noted, too, that
different cultures set distinctive norms for closeness in, e.g., speaking, business, and courting,
and that standing too close or too far away can lead to misunderstandings and
even to culture shock.
Body space III. Summarizing diverse studies,
Vrugt and Kerkstra (1984:5)
concluded that, "In interaction between strangers the interpersonal distance
between women is smaller than between men and
women."
Crowded space
I. "A persistent and popular view holds that high population density
inevitably leads to violence. This myth, which is based on rat research, applies
neither to us nor to other primates" (Waal et al. 2000:77).
Crowded space II. "This pathological togetherness
[resulting from a rat population explosion which led to killing, sexual
assaults, and cannibalism], as Calhoun [1962] described it, as well as the
attendant chaos and behavioral deviancy, led him to coin the phrase 'behavioral
sink'" (Waal et al. 2000:77).
Crowded space III.
"In some of the short-term crowding experiments conducted by others and
ourselves, monkeys were literally packed together, without much room to avoid
body contact, in a cramped space for periods of up to a few hours. No dramatic
aggression increases were measured. In fact, in my last conversation with the
late John Calhoun, he mentioned having created layers of rats on top of each
other and having been surprised at how passively they reacted" (Waal
2000:10).
Culture. In Japan, one may hand prow (i.e., face
the palm-edge of one hand vertically forward in front of the nose), and bow the
head slightly, to aplogize for crossing between two people, or intruding into
another's space to move through a crowded room. "The hand acts like the prow of
a ship cutting through water" (Morris
1994:115).
Elevator space. 1. "In choosing to
approach someone in order to push the [button on the control] panel, men and
women reacted to different signals (Hughes and Goldman 1978); men preferred to
approach people who stood with eyes averted to people who looked at them and
smiled; women, however, preferred to approach someone who looked and
smiled" (Vrugt and Kerkstra
1984:9). 2. "Chimpanzees take this withdrawal
tactic one step further: they are actually less aggressive when briefly crowded.
Again, this reflects greater [primate] emotional restraint. Their reaction is
reminiscent of people on an elevator, who reduce frictions by minimizing large
body movements, eye contact and loud vocalizations" (Waal et al. 2000:81).
Escalator
space. "Men reacted more to the person standing [immediately, i.e., just one
step behind, with the hands reaching forward on the rail so as to be visible to
the person ahead] behind them than did women" (Vrugt and Kerkstra 1984:9).
"Women seem to prefer to act as if they do not notice anything, so that unwanted
contact can be avoided. Men make it clear in their reactions that they do not
appreciate such a rapprochement" (Vrugt and Kerkstra 1984:10).
Library
space. Regardless of an "invader's" sex, men already seated at an otherwise
unoccupied table view opposites most negatively, while already seated women view
adjacents most negatively (Fisher and Byrne
1975).
Parking
space. "A study of more than 400 drivers at an Atlanta-area mall parking lot
found that motorists defend their spots instinctively" (AP, May 13, 1997; from
research published in the Journal of Applied Social Psychology, May
1997). "It's not your paranoid imagination after all: People exiting parking
spaces really do leave more slowly when you're waiting for the spot . . . . It's
called territorial behavior . . ." (AP, May 13,
1997).
Office space I. Office workers spend
the day in an average 260 square-foot (down from 1986's 275 square-foot),
usually rectangular space. Corporate downsizing and belt-tightening mean that
many staffers now find themselves working in even smaller, modular,
80-square-foot cubicles. (N.B.: For some
prehistoric context, consider that our hunter-gatherer ancestors spent their
workdays on an estimated 440-square-mile expanse of open savannah.)
Cubicles replaced the more exposed, "pool" desks which had earlier lined the
floors of cavernous group-occupied workrooms. Though maligned in Dilbert
cartoons, cubicles at least provide more privacy than the 1950s open workrooms,
and offer needed respite from visual monitoring (which is known to be stressful
to human primates).
Office space II. "German business personnel
visiting the United States see our open doors in offices and businesses as
indicative of an unusually relaxed and unbusinesslike attitude. Americans get
the feeling that the German's [sic] closed doors conceal a secretive or
conspiratorial operation" (Vargas 1986:98).
Restaurant space.
Corner and wall tables are occupied first (Eibl-Eibesfeldt
1970).
Home space I. Americans spend an
estimated 70 years indoors, mostly in the secure habitat of an average-sized,
2,000-square-foot residences called a home (from the Indo-European
root, tkei-, "settle" or "site").
(N.B.:
Because there is no counterpart in primate evolution for a life lived entirely
indoors, we bring the outdoors in. Thus, better homes and gardens include
obvious replicas, as well as subtle reminders, of the original savanna-grassland
territory, including its warmth, lighting, colors, vistas, textures, and
plants.)
Home space II. Upon re-entering our home (after
several hours of absence), we feel a peculiar need to wander about the space to
"check" for intruders. In mammals, this behavior is known as
reconnaissance: ". . . in which the animal moves round its range in a
fully alerted manner so that all its sense organs are used as much as possible,
resulting in maximal exposure to stimuli from the environment. It thus
'refreshes its memory' and keeps a check on everything in its area" [this is "a
regular activity in an already familiar environment," which does "not require
the stimulus of a strange object"] (Ewer 1968:66).
Neighborhood space. The prime directive of neighborhood space is, "Stay in your own yard." That we are terribly territorial is reflected in fences by the barriers they define. According to the American Fencing Association, 38,880 miles of chain link, 31,680 miles of wooden, and 1,440 miles of ornamental fencing are bought annually in the U.S. (N.B.: Each year Americans buy enough residential fencing to encircle the earth nearly three times.)
City space I. Biologists call the space in which primates live their home range. The home range of human hunter-gatherers (e.g., of the Kalahari Bushmen in southern Africa) spreads outward ca. 15-to-20 miles in all directions from a central home base. The home range of today's city dwelling humans includes a home base (an apartment or a house) as well, along with favored foraging territories (e.g., a shopping mall and supermarket), a juvenile nursery (i.e., a school), a sporting area (e.g., a golf course), a work space (an office building, e.g.)--and from two-to-five nocturnal drinking-and-dining spots. We spend most of our lives a. occupying these favorite spaces, and b. orbiting among them on habitually traveled pathways, sidewalks, and roads.
City space II. "Fixing Broken
Windows, a book by [Rutgers criminologist George] Kelling and co-author
Catherine Coles, became a bible for New York City's 'zero-tolerance' policy
toward abandoned cars, abandoned buildings and even graffiti. [new paragraph]
"Kelling and Coles argue that even small signs of crime and decay in a
neighborhood, such as broken windows, encourage crime by signaling that such
behavior is tolerated" (Bayles 2000: 3A).
National space. We
live in one of ca. 160 sovereign nations which together claim 54% of earth's
surface, including almost all of its land and much of its oceans, waterways, and
airspace. Over ninety percent of all nations, including the U.S., have
unresolved border disputes (see WWW.Army.mil).
Outer space. No national sovereignty rules in outer space. Those
who venture there go as envoys of the entire human race. Their quest, therefore,
must be for all mankind, and what they find should belong to all mankind.
--Lyndon Baines Johnson
U.S. politics. "Distance between two
shakers who are still connected at the hand signifies either distrust,
aloofness, or reserve. Democratic presidential candidate Michael Dukakis, often
criticized in the media for his lack of passion in his campaign style, tends to
shake hands by planting his feet and extending his right arm out to meet the
oncoming hand of the other shaker" (Blum 1988:7-4).
Neuro-notes
I. 1. In imaging studies of our brain, the neural basis of spatial
location and navigation shows activation of the right hippocampus. Travel to a
place activates the right caudate nucleus of the basal
ganglia (Maguire et al. 1998). 2. "The navigation system includes
special 'place cells' and 'direction cells' [in the hippocampus] that flicker
visibly in MRI images when a research subject tries to find his or her way
through a simulated urban environment" (Boyd 2000). 3. "A section of the
[London taxi] cabbies' brains, called the hippocampus, became enlarged during
the two years they spent learning their way around the vast, complicated
metropolis" (Boyd 2000; see PRIMATE
BRAIN, Climbing cues).
Neuro-notes II. Damage to
the right parietal lobe's angular gyrus and supra-marginal gyrus may cause
problems in our ability to use space (such as, e.g., a difficulty in dressing,
problems orienting in space, trouble drawing figures in 3D, and neglect of the
body's entire left side). Lesions in the right hemisphere's parietal lobe may
affect our spatial comprehension.
Neuro-notes III. Mirror neurons: Mirror neurons may play an important role in proxemics. Consider Leonardo Fogassi's abstract for the 2012 conference on "Mirror Neurons: New Frontiers 20 Years After Their Discovery": ". . . mirror neurons can be modulated by the distance at which the motor act is performed, suggesting a possible role in triggering different types of space-related behavioural reactions."
See also
ANGULAR DISTANCE, CONFERENCE
TABLE, LOOM, STEINZOR
EFFECT, TOUCH CUE.
Copyright 1998 - 2018 (David B. Givens/Center for
Nonverbal Studies)
News photo of U.S. Democratic presidential candidates Hillary Clinton and Barack Obama at a debate in 2008; their angular distance bespeaks the acknowledged disliking they felt as the campaign dragged on (picture credit: unknown).