FACIAL
EXPRESSION
I will often fly great
distances to meet someone face to face . . . . --Mark
H. McCormack (What They Don't Teach You at Harvard
Business School, 1984:9)
Sign.
The act of communicating a mood, attitude, opinion, feeling, or other message by contracting the muscles of the face.
Usage: The combined expressive force of our mobile chin, lip, cheek, eye, and brow muscles is without peer in the animal kingdom. Better than any body parts, our faces reveal emotions, opinions, and moods. While we learn to manipulate some expressions (see, e.g., SMILE), many unconscious facial expressions (see, e.g., LIP-POUT, TENSE-MOUTH, and TONGUE-SHOW) reflect our true feelings and hidden attitudes. Many facial expressions are universal, though most may be shaped by cultural usages and rules (see below, Culture).
Summary of facial expressions. 1. Nose:
nostril flare (arousal). 2. Lips:
grin (happiness, affiliation, contentment); grimace (fear);
lip-compression (anger, emotion, frustration); canine snarl
(disgust); lip-pout (sadness, submission, uncertainty);
lip-purse (disagree); sneer (contempt; see below, Sneer).
3. Brows: frown (anger, sadness,
concentration); brow-raise (intensity). 4.
Tongue: tongue-show (dislike, disagree). 5.
Eyelids: flashbulb eyes (surprise); widened
(excitement, surprise); narrowed (threat, disagreement); fast-blink (arousal); normal-blink
(relaxed). 6. Eyes: big pupils (arousal, fight-or-flight); small pupils (rest-and-digest); direct-gaze (affiliate,
threaten); gaze cut-off (dislike, disagree); gaze-down (submission, deception); CLEMS
(thought processing). (NOTE: See
individual entries elsewhere in The
Nonverbal Dictionary.)
Child development. ". . .
all children, regardless of cultural background, show the same maturation
process when it comes to the basic emotional expressions [e.g., of anger, fear,
and joy]" (Burgoon et al. 1989:350; see below, RESEARCH
REPORTS).
Culture. "Japanese are taught to mask negative
facial expressions with smiles and laughter and to display less facial affect
overall, leading some Westerners to consider the Japanese inscrutable (Friesen,
1972; Morsbach, 1973; Ramsey, 1983)" (Burgoon et al. 1989:193).
Embryology. The nerves and muscles that open and close our mouth
derive from the 1st pharyngeal arch, while those that constrict our
throat derive from the 3rd and 4th arches. In the disgusted or
"yuck-face," cranial VII contracts orbital muscles to narrow
our eyes, as well as corrugator and associated muscle groups to lower
our brows. (Each of these muscles and nerves derives from the 2nd
pharyngeal arch.) We may express positive, friendly, and confident moods by
dilating our eye, nose, throat, and mouth openings--or we may show
negative and anxious feelings (as well as inferiority) by constricting
them. Thus, the underlying principle of movement established in the jawless
fishes long ago remains much the same today: Unpleasant emotions and stimuli
lead cranial nerves to constrict our eye, nose, mouth, and throat openings,
while more pleasant sensations widen our facial orifices to incoming
cues.
Evolution I. In the Jurassic period, mammalian
faces gradually became more mobile (and far more expressive) than the rigid
faces of reptiles. Muscles which earlier controlled the pharyngeal
arches (i.e., the primitive "gill" openings) came to move mammalian
lips, muzzles, scalps, and external ear flaps. Nerve links from the emotional
limbic system to the facial muscles--routed through the
brain stem's facial and trigeminal nerves (cranial VII and
V)--enable us to express joy, fear, sadness,
surprise, interest, anger, and disgust
today.
Evolution II. That a nose-stinging whiff of ammonium carbonate can
cause our face to close up in disgust shows how facial expression,
smell, and taste are linked. The connection traces back to the ancient muscles
and nerves of the pharyngeal arches of our remote Silurian ancestors. Pharyngeal
arches were part of the feeding and breathing apparatus of the jawless fishes;
sea water was pumped in and out of the early pharynx through a series of gill
slits at the animal's head end. Each arch contained a visceral nerve
and a somatic muscle to close the gill opening in case dangerous
chemicals were sensed. Very early in Nonverbal
World, pharyngeal arches were programmed to constrict in
response to noxious tastes and smells.
Gag reflex. The ancient
pattern is reflected in our faces today. In infants, e.g., a bitter taste shows
in lowered brows, narrowed eyes, and a protruded
tongue--the yuck-face expression pictured on poison-warning labels. A bad
flavor causes baby to seal off her throat and oral cavity as cranial nerves
IX and X activate the pharyngeal gag reflex. Cranial
V depresses the lower jaw to expel the unpleasant mouthful (then closes
it to keep food out), as cranial XII protrudes the
tongue.
Gender differences. "Not surprisingly, women have a
general superiority over men when it comes to decoding facial expressions . . ."
(Burgoon et al. 1989:360).
Mimicking. Research indicates that
mimicking another's face elicits empathy (Berstein et al., 2000).
Primatology. 1. In our closest primate relatives, the Old World
monkeys and apes, the following facial expressions have been identified: alert
face, bared-teeth gecker face, frowning bared-teeth scream face, lip-smacking
face, pout face, protruded-lips face, relaxed face, relaxed open-mouth face,
silent bared-teeth face, staring bared-teeth scream face, staring open-mouth
face, teeth-chattering face, and tense-mouth face (Van Hooff 1967). 2.
"Andrew (1963, 1965) held that facial expressions were originally natural
physical response to stimuli. As these responses became endowed with the
function of communication, they survived the various stages of evolution and
were passed along to man" (Izard 1971:38; cf. NONVERBAL
INDEPENDENCE).
Sneer. In the sneer, buccinator
muscles (innervated by lower buccal branches of the facial nerve) contract to
draw the lip corners sideward to produce a sneering "dimple" in the cheeks (the
sneer may also be accompanied by a scornful, upward eye-roll). From videotape
studies of nearly 700 married couples in sessions discussing their emotional
relationships with each other, University of Washington psychologist, John
Gottman has found the sneer expression (even fleeting episodes of the cue) to be
a "potent signal" for predicting the likelihood of future marital disintegration
(Bates and Cleese 2001). In this regard, the sneer may be decoded as an
unconscious sign of contempt.
RESEARCH
REPORTS: So closely is emotion tied to facial expression that it
is hard to imagine one without the other. 1. The first major
scientific study of facial communication was published by Charles Darwin in
1872. Darwin concluded that many expressions and their meanings (e.g., for
astonishment, shame, fear, horror,
pride, hatred, wrath, love, joy,
guilt, anxiety, shyness, and modesty) are
universal: "I have endeavoured to show in considerable detail that all the chief
expressions exhibited by man are the same throughout the world" (Darwin
1872:355). 2. Sylvan S. Tomkins found eight "basic" facial
emotions: surprise, interest, joy, rage,
fear, disgust, shame and anguish (Tomkins
1962; Carroll Izard proposed a similar set of eight [Izard 1977]).
3. Studies indicate that the facial expressions of happiness, sadness, anger, fear,
surprise, disgust, and interest are universal across cultures
(Ekman and Friesen 1971). 4. ". . . the emotion process includes a motor
component subserved by innate neural programs which give rise to universal
facial patterns. These patterns are subject to repression, suppression, and
other consequences of socialization during childhood and adolescence" (Izard
1971:78).
E-Commentary I: The face entranced. "I have observed that when
a woman absent-mindedly knots a lock of her hair on a finger or twists her ring
on her finger, she often displays a trance
like facial expression--i.e., her glance seems to look
far away, her face has no expression, the right and left sides of her face are
more symmetrical, she slows or loses her eye-blink, her pupils dilate, she
half-opens her mouth as her chin falls down (her jaw appears relaxed),
and her body appears fairly passive or motionless. I have seen the same
nonverbal pattern in men, as well." --Dr. Marco Pacori, Institute of Analogic
Psychology, Milano, Italy (3/29/00 9:17:37 AM Pacific Standard Time)
Neuro-notes III. 1. "The facial nucleus [of the albino rat] contains numerous medium-caliber, intensely immunoreactive dynorphin fibers, especially in the intermediate subdivision of the nucleus . . ." (Fallon and Ciofi 1990:31). 2. "The functions of these projections are unknown, but it is likely that dynorphin and enkephalin would modulate motor neurons enervating the facial musculature, especially those in the intermediate division controlling the zygomatic, platysma and mentalis muscles" (Fallon and Ciofi 1990:31-2).
Neuro-notes IV. Mirror neurons: Mirror-neuron properties for perceiving and creating facial expressions are found in the inferior parietal lobe (IPL: involved in tool usage and perception of emotions in facial expressions), frontal operculum (fO: involved in cognitive and perceptual motor processing), and premotor cortex (involved in spatial and sensory guidance of movement, and in understanding the movements of others) (Haxby and Gobbini 2011:101). "Facial expressions contain both motor and emotional components. The inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and posterior parietal cortex have been considered to compose a mirror neuron system (MNS) for the motor components of facial expressions, while the amygdala and insula may represent an 'additional' MNS for emotional states" [van der Gaag, C., Minderaa, R. B., and C. Keysers, "Facial Expressions: What the Mirror Neuron System Can and Cannot Tell Us," Social Neuroscience, 2007; 2(3-4):179-222].
Neuro-notes V. Mirror neurons: The amygdala responds most strongly to viewed facial expressions of fear, while the anterior insula responds to viewed facial expressions of disgust (Haxby and Gobbini 2011:102). "The activity in these areas suggests that understanding the emotional meaning of expressions involves evoking the emotion itself--a simulation or mirroring of another's emotion that is analogous to the putative role of the hMNS [human mirror neuron system] in simulating or mirroring the motor actions of another" (Haxby and Gobbini 2011:102).
Neuro-notes VI. Mirror neurons: Learning influences young children's mirror neurons for reading facial expressions. Consider Pier Francesco Ferrari's abstract for the 2012 conference on "Mirror Neurons: New Frontiers 20 Years After Their Discovery": "In the course of early development mirror neurons for face gestures are strongly influenced by social interactions and can be shaped by the affective feedback of the caregiver."Neuro-notes VIII. Mirror neurons: "Accordingly, empathy is a routine involuntary process, as demonstrated by electromyographic studies of invisible muscle contractions in people's faces in response to pictures of human facial expressions. These reactions are fully automated, occurring even when we are unaware of what we saw (Dimberg et al. 2000 [Dimberg, U., Thunberg, M., and K. Elmehed (2000). "Unconscious Facial Reactions to Emotional Facial Expressions," in Psychological Science, Vol. 11, pp. 86-89])" [p. 59; source: de Waal, Frans B. M. (2007). Chapter 3: "The 'Russian Doll' Model of Empathy and Imitation," in Braten, Stein (Ed.), On Being Moved: From Mirror Neurons to Empathy (2007; Amsterdam: John Benjamins), pp. 49-70].
See also BLANK FACE.
Copyright 1998 - 2020 (David B. Givens & John White) /Center for Nonverbal Studies)
Detail of photo showing contraction of depressor anguli oris (pulling down lip corners [in sadness]) and mentalis muscles (dimpling chin [in lip-pout]; mentalis protrudes and everts the lower lip; the dimpled-chin expression correlates well with doubt, disdain, and negative emotions [picture copyright 1949 by The Williams and Wilkins Co.])