MUSIC
Usage: Music produces a highly evocative, emotional message through harmony, melody, rhythm, and timbre.
Amusia. "Cases of amusia, i.e., loss of ability to produce or comprehend music--an abnormality as regards music analogous to aphasia as regards the faculty of speech--conclusively demonstrate that the musical faculties do not depend on the speech faculty [i.e., one may suffer from amusia without aphasia, and vice versa, though some may suffer from both]" (Reiling 1999:218).
Anthropology. So diverse are the world's musical "languages" that some sociocultural anthropologists specialize entirely in ethnomusicology.
Head bangers. 1. In a study of early-childhood head bangers,
mothers described their children as ". . . prone to rhythmic activity in
response to musical stimuli" (De Lissovoy 1962:56; see SELF-TOUCH, Neuro-notes). 2. ". . . all of
the [33] subjects had a history of other rhythmic activities, such as head or
body rolling, prior to the head banging" (De Lissovoy 1962:56). 3. Girls
head banged 19-to-52, while boys head banged 26-to-121, times per minute (De
Lissovoy 1962).
Lullaby. "A Chinese lullaby is just as soothing
to a child as a German song or any other. When listening to lullabyes, breathing
becomes shallow and regular like that of a sleeping person. The characteristics
of this form of breathing are also in the structure of the lullaby"
(Eibl-Eibesfeldt 1970:439).
Prehistory. "During the last two
decades many investigators--Kussmaul, Stumpf, Preyer, Oppenheim, Knoblouch,
Charcot, etc.--have conclusively demonstrated that the musical faculty is older
than that of speech; that music is a primary and simple phenomenon, while speech
is secondary and complex" (Reiling 1999:218).
Symphony. "The highs
and lows of emotional experiences are touched in an ever-changing pattern that
cannot be experienced in everyday life" (Eibl-Eibesfeldt 1970:440).
The question: "Why is the sound of music important in
human courtship?"
Ethnographic
background. Thousands of Endfesters arrived; they were
17-to-30 years old, mostly unmarried, urban, white,
heterosexual fans of alternative rock music. Showing up in groups of 2, 3, and
4--all-male, all-female, or mixed female-and-male--they were excited and
ready to rock.
Adornment. Endfesters dressed to show
off essential male or female gender cues, and to display individuality,
personality, and allegiance to the alternative lifestyle. Fans wore
identity-proclaiming belts, boots,
bracelets, caps and hats, cut-through jeans, dark glasses, earrings, necklaces, foot-revealing
sandals, conspicuously displayed underwear, idiosyncratic watches, and very visible tatoos. Band members dressed in black (see COLOR
CUE, BLACK).
Hair. Endfesters delighted in the display of head hair (see HAIR
CUE). The most outstanding display was a man's
well-groomed, magenta topknot, projecting stiffly above his close-cropped hair's
jet-black sidewalls. Clearly visible at distances of 100 yards, his
nonverbal message was aposematic with the coloration and contrast of a stinging wasp or bee:
"Danger, danger, danger!" (see HAT,
Cap III).
Media. In poster photographs published in the August 5,
2000 Bremerton Sun newspaper, blank-faced band members of Third
Eye Blind lean away sideward to show defiant attitudes.
Unsmiling, blank-faced members of 3 Doors Down stare menacingly, straight ahead
(see EYE
CONTACT, Usage). Unsmiling, blank-faced members of Papa Roach
pose with their heads tilted sideward in a posture popularized by the method
actor, James Dean (see SHOULDER-SHRUG, Media).
Motion I. Because both our auditory and
vestibular senses involve sensors housed within the ears, music powerfully
suggests movement. The phrase "rock and roll," e.g., is a vestibular metaphor
for the sound of music. The loud rock music at Endfest joined listeners as
psychic "fellow travelers," and thus enhanced the rapport
of strangers in the crowd.
Motion II. Set to music, Endfester body movements took on a more palpable, emotional appeal. Submerged in
the loud electronic beat, group isopraxism bourgeoned and enhanced as
well.
Emotion I. Not only were the rock-music lyrics spoken in
heightened emotional voice tones, but the guitar and organ sounds, which mimic
the sound-range of the human voice itself, also "spoke" to the crowd's feelings
and moods.
Emotion II. Singers used
aggressive, angry voice tones to scream and shout--in order to target negative
emotion centers of the brain's amygdala. Threatening sounds, venomous shrieks, and
harmful, biting words put into the summer air, very amplified, from tensed
throats, touched off feelings of group belonging and "togetherness" via the
biological principle of aggression-out. Just as monkeys mob outsiders, by
sharing dislike for and distrust of mainstream (i.e., non-alternative) values,
Endfesters became a close-knit group in which courtship could take
place.
Speech. Amplified (16 coaxial cables fed into
the main stage), the words of the rock musicians fully engaged listeners'
brains. Addressed to the crowd through eye contact, listeners felt emotionally
and personally connected--not only to the singers but to each other as
well.
Sound. In mating rituals throughout the world, auditory cues
play a tactile role as they pave the way for physical touching itself (see AUDITORY
CUE, Courtship).
Touch.
In the crowds surrounding Stage A, men formed ad hoc combat circles and pushed
each other to and fro, with their hands held in aggressively pronated (i.e., palm-down) positions, as Harvey Danger played its hit
song, "Flagpole Sitta." Surrounded by women, the pushing and shoving was not
unlike the ritual clash of elk antlers in mating season.
Neuro-notes I. Research on amusia suggests ". . . that there is only one musical center in the cerebrum, and that it is situated in the anterior two-thirds of the first temporal convolution and in the anterior half of the second temporal convolution of the left lobe, i.e., in front of the [speech-comprehension] center of Wernicke" (Reiling 1999:218).
Neuro-notes II. "Larionoff has made numerous ingenious experiments
on dogs, with a view of defining the localization of the auditory centers, and
has come to the following conclusions: There are several sensory musical centers
situated in the posterior halves of the hemispheres, and several motor centers
situated in the anterior halves of the hemispheres of the cerebrum. Of the
sensory, two tone centers are situated in the temporal lobes, and one optic
center, for the reading of notes, situated alongside of the center for ordinary
reading, in the gyrus angularis. The motor center of notewriting probably
develops alongside of the center for ordinary writing, in the second frontal
convolution. The singing center is situated a little behind the motor center of
speech of Broca, in the third frontal convolution, and is otherwise known as the
center of Krause. The motor center presiding over the functions of performing on
various instruments develops on exercising, in the anterior part of the central
convolution alongside of the motor center of note writing. The center for
playing wind instruments is developed in the region governing the movements of
the lips, a little above the center of Krause . . ." (Reiling
1999:218).
Neuro-notes III. PET studies of listening to familiar
melodies show involvement of the right superior temporal cortex, the right
inferior temporal cortex, and the supplementary motor area (Halpern and Zatorre
1999). Retrieval of a familiar melody activates the right frontal area and right
superior temporal gyrus (Halpern and Zatorre 1999). No significant activity was
observed in the left temporal lobe (Halpern and Zatorre 1999). "It is concluded
that areas of right auditory association cortex, together with right and left
frontal cortices, are implicated in imagery for familiar melodies" (Halpern and
Zatorre 1999). "Retrieval from musical semantic memory is mediated by structures
in the right frontal lobe" (Halpern and Zatorre 1999).
Neuro-notes IV. Mirror neurons: According to Istvan Molnar-Szakacs and Katie Overy, ". . . music can invoke motor representations of emotions by recruiting the insula, a neural relay between the limbic and motor systems. Action, language and music appear to share neural resources, and we have proposed that common features governing the use and function of these means of communication may be represented within the fronto-parietal mirror neuron system" ("Music and Mirror Neurons: From Motion to 'E'motion," in Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2006, Vol. 1, Issue 3, pp. 235-41).
Neuro-notes V. Singing and gesturing: Around the world, music is vocal (e.g., singing and chanting) and gestural (manual, e.g., clapping, finger-snapping, and playing varied musical instruments with the hands). Why vocal (laryngeal) and gestural (pectoral) articulations are so important in music may be explained, in part, by the following. Muscles that today move the human larynx and pectoral girdle evolved from hypobranchial muscles that originally opened the mouths and gill openings of ancient fishes. Paleocircuits that mediate our laryngeal and pectoral movements are connected in the posterior hindbrain and anterior spinal cord (Bass and Chagnaud 2013). The sonic properties of these bodily regions (vocalizing and pectoral vibration, respectively) were recruited for social signaling in a watery world. The sounds were basically "assertion displays" used to announce a sender's physical presence, oftentimes in courtship to attract mates and repel rivals. Controlled by branchial muscles, these body parts were more easily aroused to produce vibratory sounds than were parts controlled by other-than-branchial nerves. In primates, the pectoral movements became visual signals, which in humans are called gestures.See also DANCE, TONE OF VOICE.
Copyright 1998 - 2022 (David B. Givens/Center for
Nonverbal Studies)
Picture credit: unknown