MUSIC

Two Players Playing


Music hath charms to soothe a savage beast,

To soften rocks, or bend a knotted oak
. --Congreve (The Mourning Bride, I, 1)


Auditory signals
. A usually pleasing, sequential arrangement of vocal or instrumental sounds.

Usage: Music produces a highly evocative, emotional message through harmony, melody, rhythm, and timbre.

Amusia. "Cases of amusia, i.e., loss of ability to produce or comprehend music--an abnormality as regards music analogous to aphasia as regards the faculty of speech--conclusively demonstrate that the musical faculties do not depend on the speech faculty [i.e., one may suffer from amusia without aphasia, and vice versa, though some may suffer from both]" (Reiling 1999:218).

Anthropology. So diverse are the world's musical "languages" that some sociocultural anthropologists specialize entirely in ethnomusicology.

Head bangers. 1. In a study of early-childhood head bangers, mothers described their children as ". . . prone to rhythmic activity in response to musical stimuli" (De Lissovoy 1962:56; see SELF-TOUCH, Neuro-notes). 2. ". . . all of the [33] subjects had a history of other rhythmic activities, such as head or body rolling, prior to the head banging" (De Lissovoy 1962:56). 3. Girls head banged 19-to-52, while boys head banged 26-to-121, times per minute (De Lissovoy 1962).

Lullaby. "A Chinese lullaby is just as soothing to a child as a German song or any other. When listening to lullabyes, breathing becomes shallow and regular like that of a sleeping person. The characteristics of this form of breathing are also in the structure of the lullaby" (Eibl-Eibesfeldt 1970:439).

Prehistory. "During the last two decades many investigators--Kussmaul, Stumpf, Preyer, Oppenheim, Knoblouch, Charcot, etc.--have conclusively demonstrated that the musical faculty is older than that of speech; that music is a primary and simple phenomenon, while speech is secondary and complex" (Reiling 1999:218).

Symphony. "The highs and lows of emotional experiences are touched in an ever-changing pattern that cannot be experienced in everyday life" (Eibl-Eibesfeldt 1970:440).


FIELD NOTES: To study the special role music plays in human courtship, CNS conducted field observations at an outdoor rock concert--Endfest 2000--held on Saturday, August 5, 2000, on the Kitsap Peninsula, west of Seattle, Washington, USA.

The question: "Why is the sound of music important in human courtship?"

Ethnographic background. Thousands of Endfesters arrived; they were 17-to-30 years old, mostly unmarried, urban, white, heterosexual fans of alternative rock music. Showing up in groups of 2, 3, and 4--all-male, all-female, or mixed female-and-male--they were excited and ready to rock.

Adornment. Endfesters dressed to show off essential male or female gender cues, and to display individuality, personality, and allegiance to the alternative lifestyle. Fans wore identity-proclaiming belts, boots, bracelets, caps and hats, cut-through jeans, dark glasses, earrings, necklaces, foot-revealing sandals, conspicuously displayed underwear, idiosyncratic watches, and very visible tatoos. Band members dressed in black (see COLOR CUE, BLACK).

Hair. Endfesters delighted in the display of head hair (see HAIR CUE). The most outstanding display was a man's well-groomed, magenta topknot, projecting stiffly above his close-cropped hair's jet-black sidewalls. Clearly visible at distances of 100 yards, his nonverbal message was aposematic with the coloration and contrast of a stinging wasp or bee: "Danger, danger, danger!" (see HAT, Cap III).

Media. In poster photographs published in the August 5, 2000 Bremerton Sun newspaper, blank-faced band members of Third Eye Blind lean away sideward to show defiant attitudes. Unsmiling, blank-faced members of 3 Doors Down stare menacingly, straight ahead (see EYE CONTACT, Usage). Unsmiling, blank-faced members of Papa Roach pose with their heads tilted sideward in a posture popularized by the method actor, James Dean (see SHOULDER-SHRUG, Media).


Motion I. Because both our auditory and vestibular senses involve sensors housed within the ears, music powerfully suggests movement. The phrase "rock and roll," e.g., is a vestibular metaphor for the sound of music. The loud rock music at Endfest joined listeners as psychic "fellow travelers," and thus enhanced the rapport of strangers in the crowd.

Motion II. Set to music, Endfester body movements took on a more palpable, emotional appeal. Submerged in the loud electronic beat, group isopraxism bourgeoned and enhanced as well.

Emotion I. Not only were the rock-music lyrics spoken in heightened emotional voice tones, but the guitar and organ sounds, which mimic the sound-range of the human voice itself, also "spoke" to the crowd's feelings and moods.

Emotion II. Singers used aggressive, angry voice tones to scream and shout--in order to target negative emotion centers of the brain's amygdala. Threatening sounds, venomous shrieks, and harmful, biting words put into the summer air, very amplified, from tensed throats, touched off feelings of group belonging and "togetherness" via the biological principle of aggression-out. Just as monkeys mob outsiders, by sharing dislike for and distrust of mainstream (i.e., non-alternative) values, Endfesters became a close-knit group in which courtship could take place.

Speech. Amplified (16 coaxial cables fed into the main stage), the words of the rock musicians fully engaged listeners' brains. Addressed to the crowd through eye contact, listeners felt emotionally and personally connected--not only to the singers but to each other as well.

Sound. In mating rituals throughout the world, auditory cues play a tactile role as they pave the way for physical touching itself (see AUDITORY CUE, Courtship).

Touch. In the crowds surrounding Stage A, men formed ad hoc combat circles and pushed each other to and fro, with their hands held in aggressively pronated (i.e., palm-down) positions, as Harvey Danger played its hit song, "Flagpole Sitta." Surrounded by women, the pushing and shoving was not unlike the ritual clash of elk antlers in mating season.


Neuro-notes I. Research on amusia suggests ". . . that there is only one musical center in the cerebrum, and that it is situated in the anterior two-thirds of the first temporal convolution and in the anterior half of the second temporal convolution of the left lobe, i.e., in front of the [speech-comprehension] center of Wernicke" (Reiling 1999:218).

Neuro-notes II. "Larionoff has made numerous ingenious experiments on dogs, with a view of defining the localization of the auditory centers, and has come to the following conclusions: There are several sensory musical centers situated in the posterior halves of the hemispheres, and several motor centers situated in the anterior halves of the hemispheres of the cerebrum. Of the sensory, two tone centers are situated in the temporal lobes, and one optic center, for the reading of notes, situated alongside of the center for ordinary reading, in the gyrus angularis. The motor center of notewriting probably develops alongside of the center for ordinary writing, in the second frontal convolution. The singing center is situated a little behind the motor center of speech of Broca, in the third frontal convolution, and is otherwise known as the center of Krause. The motor center presiding over the functions of performing on various instruments develops on exercising, in the anterior part of the central convolution alongside of the motor center of note writing. The center for playing wind instruments is developed in the region governing the movements of the lips, a little above the center of Krause . . ." (Reiling 1999:218).

Neuro-notes III. PET studies of listening to familiar melodies show involvement of the right superior temporal cortex, the right inferior temporal cortex, and the supplementary motor area (Halpern and Zatorre 1999). Retrieval of a familiar melody activates the right frontal area and right superior temporal gyrus (Halpern and Zatorre 1999). No significant activity was observed in the left temporal lobe (Halpern and Zatorre 1999). "It is concluded that areas of right auditory association cortex, together with right and left frontal cortices, are implicated in imagery for familiar melodies" (Halpern and Zatorre 1999). "Retrieval from musical semantic memory is mediated by structures in the right frontal lobe" (Halpern and Zatorre 1999).

Neuro-notes IV. Mirror neurons: According to Istvan Molnar-Szakacs and Katie Overy, ". . . music can invoke motor representations of emotions by recruiting the insula, a neural relay between the limbic and motor systems. Action, language and music appear to share neural resources, and we have proposed that common features governing the use and function of these means of communication may be represented within the fronto-parietal mirror neuron system" ("Music and Mirror Neurons: From Motion to 'E'motion," in Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2006, Vol. 1, Issue 3, pp. 235-41).

Neuro-notes V. Singing and gesturing: Around the world, music is vocal (e.g., singing and chanting) and gestural (manual, e.g., clapping, finger-snapping, and playing varied musical instruments with the hands). Why vocal (laryngeal) and gestural (pectoral) articulations are so important in music may be explained, in part, by the following. Muscles that today move the human larynx and pectoral girdle evolved from hypobranchial muscles that originally opened the mouths and gill openings of ancient fishes. Paleocircuits that mediate our laryngeal and pectoral movements are connected in the posterior hindbrain and anterior spinal cord (Bass and Chagnaud 2013). The sonic properties of these bodily regions (vocalizing and pectoral vibration, respectively) were recruited for social signaling in a watery world. The sounds were basically "assertion displays" used to announce a sender's physical presence, oftentimes in courtship to attract mates and repel rivals. Controlled by branchial muscles, these body parts were more easily aroused to produce vibratory sounds than were parts controlled by other-than-branchial nerves. In primates, the pectoral movements became visual signals, which in humans are called gestures.

See also DANCE, TONE OF VOICE.


LULLABY

Musical voice tones. Usually maternal, soothing vocal tones and prosodic voice qualities softly sung to infants, young children and often to adults in the early stages of courting (see COURTSHIP).

Usage. Sung at close quarters, lullabyes may be used to calm the nerves of physically or mentally upset youngsters, helping them relax and transition from alertness to sleep.

Word origin. English lullaby comes to Middle English from "lullen" (to lull) and "bye-bye" (away, to bed, sleep).

Culture. Though lullaby words differ variously in Iceland, Ireland, Italy and the Philippines, nonverbal sounds of lullabies may be culturally universal. Higher-than-normal pitches and pitch ranges; hypnotic, rhythmic repetition; and slower, softer tempo. The singer's lips may be visibly closer to the listener's ear, as if in readiness to kiss (see INTENTION CUE).

See also LANGUAGE ORIGIN - STEP 15, Serenade.


SINGING

Musical sign. Non-verbal vocalizations, spoken words and manual gestures set to the emotional, highly evocative harmony, melody, rhythm and timbre of music.



Emotional range. Human song may express the entirety of felt emotion: agreement, anger, certainty, control, disagreement, disgust, disliking, embarrassment, fear, happiness, hate, interest, liking, love, sadness, shame, surprise and uncertainty.



Usage. Around the world, music is both vocal (e.g., chanting, humming, and singing) and gestural (manual, e.g., clapping, finger-snapping, and playing varied musical instruments with the hands).


Evolution.  Beginning with the use of speech ca. 200 thousand years ago, the human voice became increasingly melodic, harmonious, and oratorical. The rationale for vocal softness and melody most certainly involved serenading in courtship (see below, Serenades).  As it became more verbally linguistic, human courtship signaling likely favored vocal
tenderness over harshness.  The former voice quality is contact-inviting while the latter promotes distance.

Vocal music. Unlike the wooden or metal tubes of a pipe organ, the human windpipe is pliable and protean in its ability to change shape. Encased in cartilage, the vibrating vocal folds produce sounds modified by elastic, membranous tissues and supple ligaments, further modified within mobile, mucus-lined pharyngeal, nasal and oral chambers of the head.  The
 musicality of human voices is processed in the planum temporale, a cortical auditory area found only in great apes and Homo.

Prosody. Linguists call the quasi-musical qualities of human speech “prosody.”  English prosody comes from Greek prosoidia, “song sung to music” or “accent.”  Linguistic prosody includes accentuation, phrasing, rhythm, stress and the tonal qualities of speech.  On the non-verbal side, prosody includes the duration, muscular tension  and rhythm of hand movements that accompany words.  Vocal and gestural prosody play important roles in the production and perception of human communication.  Through them we detect emotions such as happiness, sadness, anger, fear and uncertainty in utterance and body movement.



Duets.  In a duet, two singers join in song and feel an emotional closeness though the non-verbal principle of isopraxism or “same behavior.”  Singing in tandem doubles the force of a comical, romantic or tragic performance.  Among great love duets in opera are those of
“Liebesnacht,” “La Boheme” and “Andrea Chenier.”



Fight songs.  Football, rugby and soccer fans may erupt in song to show allegiance and exhort their teams to win.  Just as they may sit together and wear the same team colors, fans may simultaneously sing the same words to bond  and band together as a force to be reckoned with.  Famous fight songs include "Eye of the Tiger," "We Will Rock You" and "Na Na Hey Hey Kiss Him Goodbye."

Serenades.  In classic form, a serenade is an evening song sung to attract a lover in courtship.  Its non-verbal traits include musical voice tones, and palm-up hand gestures reached out to the listening loved one.  A famous version is Luciano Pavarotti's operatic rendition of "Serenade," delivered with exquisite vocal melody and lyrical motions of his fingers
and hands.



Voice and gesture.  That vocal (laryngeal) and gestural (pectoral) articulations are both important in singing may be explained, in part, by the fact muscles in charge of the human larynx and pectoral girdle evolved from hypobranchial muscles that originally opened the mouths and gill openings of ancient fishes.  Paleocircuits that mediate our laryngeal and pectoral movements are connected in the posterior hindbrain and anterior spinal cord (Bass and Chagnaud 2013).

Vocal and pectoral vibrations.  Human singing has incredibly ancient vertebrate roots, which may explain why song is such a compelling non-verbal venue today.  The sonic properties of vocalizing and pectoral
vibration were recruited ca. 420 mya (Early Silurian or earlier) for social signaling in a watery world.  The sounds were basically "assertion displays" used to announce a sender's physical presence, attract mates and repel rivals.



See also DANCE, TONE OF VOICE, WHISTLE
*
YouTube Video: Take five to enjoy "Take Five" (1966).

YouTube Video: Something shorter still . . .

YouTube Video: Watch our son, Aaron Huffman (1972-2016; in the light blue shirt), play bass guitar for his Gold Record winning band, Harvey Danger . . .

Copyright 1998 - 2022 (David B. Givens/Center for Nonverbal Studies)
Picture credit: unknown