Laugh and the world laughs with you,
Weep and you weep alone. . . . --Ella Wheeler
Wilcox, Solitude
(1883)
Then she laughed. It was almost a racking laugh. It shook her as the wind shakes a tree. I thought there was puzzlement in it, not exactly surprise, but as if a new idea had been added to something already known and it didn't fit. Then I thought that was too much to get out of a laugh. --Philip Marlowe, describing Eddie Mars' wife (The Big Sleep, 1939:196)
We never read of His laughing, though I am sure he did.
--Billy Graham (1955:v)
Your mouth was twisted open, your tongue was
stuck out halfway, your lips were pulled back and your nostrils were flared.
Though you felt pleasure, your face suggested pain. Your cheeks turned red
and you doubled over, gasping. Your stomach, chest and ribs ached. You were
helpless, unable even to speak. --Robert Brody, "Anatomy of a Laugh"
(1983:43)
Rhythmic vocalization. 1. Human laughter varies
greatly in form, duration, and loudness (see, e.g., Ruch 1993, Ruch and Ekman
2001). A common form of laughter includes sudden, decrescendo (i.e., strong
onset to soft ending), forced-expiration bursts of breathy vowel sounds (e.g.,
"hee-hee," "heh-heh," "ha-ha," or "ho-ho-ho") given in response to
embarrassment, excitement, or humor. 2. In extreme form, an
involuntary spasm of the respiratory muscles, accompanied by an open-mouth
smile, flared nostrils,
tearing
eyes, facial flushing, and forward bowing
motions of the head and torso. 3. In mean-spirited form,
laughter (esp. group laughter) may be directed at enemies and persons with whom
we disagree or dislike, as a form of aggression-out. This
mocking-aggressive laughter resembles the group-mobbing vocalizations of
higher primates.
Usage: To laugh is human ("Man is the only animal who laughs," noted
the French philosopher Henri Bergson; but see below, Primatology I).
Chemically, according to some researchers, laughter provides relief from stress
by releasing pain-killing, euphoria-producing endorphins,
enkephalins, dopamine, noradrenaline, and
adrenaline. Socially, laughter binds us as friendly allies united
against outsiders, and against forces beyond our control. Psychologically, the
comic laugh (in response, e.g., to funny jokes, puns, and satire) is a recent
development perhaps linked to the evolution of speech
(see below, Speech).
Anatomy. 1. Diverse facial,
jaw, and throat muscles are involved in the laugh, including levator labii
superioris, risorius, mentalis, depressor anguli oris
(the "frown" muscle), orbicularis oris, buccinator, and
depressor labii inferioris (Ruch 1993). 2. Laughter may be
accompanied by a general lowering of muscle tonus and an increase in bodily
relaxation, leading one, e.g., to "collapse in laughter" (see Ruch 1993).
3. In laughing, the abdominal muscles and diaphragm contract in a
respiratory "fit," not unlike sneezing or crying. Zygomatic and risorious
muscles of the face contract in a grimacing smile; mandibular muscles may
rhythmically contract as the lower jaw quivers. In a belly laugh, heartbeat
accelerates, blood pressure rises, and vocal cords may uncontrollably
vibrate.
Conscious control. "Does the low level of conscious
control that we have over our own laughter reflect the typical level of control
that non-human animals have over their own species-typical vocalizations?"
(Provine 1996).
Contagious laughter. "Consider the bizarre events
of the 1962 outbreak of contagious laughter in Tanganyika. What began as an
isolated fit of laughter (and sometimes crying) in a group of 12- to 18-year-old
schoolgirls rapidly rose to epidemic proportions. Contagious laughter propagated
from one individual to the next, eventually infecting adjacent communities. The
epidemic was so severe that it required the closing of schools. It lasted for
six months" (Provine 1996).
Exhilaration. Laughter is frequently
associated with--and thus may be a sign
of--the emotion of exhilaration (Ruch 1993). According to
Ruch (1993), exhilaration is a "pleasurable, relaxed excitation" which begins
with a "sudden and intense increase in cheerfulness, followed by a more or less
pronounced plateau and a prolonged fading out of the emotional
tone."
Life history. The human laugh is partly learned, partly
familial, and so highly contagious that we readily respond to televised "canned
laughter" (see MEDIA, "TV II"). As infants, we laugh reflexively
near the 10th week of life. When very old we may cackle, as the larynx
becomes inelastic with age.
Literature. 1. "There was
laughter of warriors, voices rang pleasant, words were cheerful."
--Beowulf. 2. "And Laughter, holding both his sides." --John
Milton (L'Allegro; ca. 1630).
Media. According to Esquire magazine, more than
anything else, women want men to make them laugh (Spokesman-Review, Feb.
7, 1999).
Primatology I. Stimulated by the mammalian
brain, laughter has much in common with animal calls. Gorillas and
chimps "laugh," e.g. (i.e., give breathy, panting vocalizations), when tickled
or playfully chased.
Primatology II. "It is noteworthy that
chimpanzee laughter occurs almost exclusively during physical contact, or during
the threat of such contact, during chasing games, wrestling or tickling. (The
individual being chased laughs the most.) Although people laugh when tickled,
most adult human laughter occurs during conversation, typically in the absence
of physical contact" (Provine 1996).
Solitary laughter. "In the
absence of stimulating media (television, radio or books), people are about 30
times more likely to laugh when they are in a social situation than when they
are alone" (Provine 1996).
Speech. 1. "One of the key
features of natural laughter is its placement in speech. Laughter is not
randomly scattered throughout the speech stream. The speaker and the audience
seldom interrupt the phrase structure of speech with laughter. In our sample of
1,200 laughs there were only eight interruptions of speech by laughter, all of
them by the speaker. Thus a speaker may say 'You are going where? . . . ha-ha,'
but rarely 'You are going . . . ha-ha . . . where?' The occurrence of laughter
during pauses at the end of phrases suggests that a lawful and probably
neurologically based process governs the placement of laughter in speech--a
process in which speech has priority access to the single vocalization channel.
The strong and orderly relationship between laughter and speech is akin to
punctuation in written communication (and is called the punctuation effect)"
(Provine 1996). 2. ". . . the average speaker laughs about 46 percent
more often than the audience" (Provine 1996).
RESEARCH REPORTS: 1. ". . . it is scarcely possible to point out any difference between the tear-stained face of a person after a paroxysm of excessive laughter and after a bitter crying-fit" (Darwin 1872:207). 2. Laughing strengthens bonds of comradeship (Van Hooff 1967:59). 3. Laughter is more social than humorous (Van Hooff 1967:59). 4. Our laugh resembles the great ape's relaxed open-mouth face (esp., its "rhythmic, low-pitched staccato vocalizations and . . . boisterous body movements" (Van Hooff 1967:60). 5. "For example, they [deaf-and-blind-born children] smile and laugh as we do when they are happy and emit the correct sounds when they do so" (Eibl-Eibesfeldt 1971:12). 6. People in good spirits may laugh 100-to-400 times a day (Fry 1983). 7. Human laughter "seldom exceeds 7 seconds" (Ruch 1993). 8. Laughter may be vocal or voiceless, may include all vowel and many consonant possibilities; it frequently begins with an initial "h" sound, most usually as "he-he," grading into "ha-ha" (Ruch 1993). 9. Robert Provine, who studied 1,200 bouts of laughter in malls and public places, characterized the verbal remarks the laughing accompanied as "not funny" (Angier 1996). 10. Provine found that a. laugh vocalizations last about 75 milliseconds, separated by rests of 210 milliseconds; b. average speakers laugh 46% more than listeners; c. male speakers laugh only slightly more than male listeners; d. female speakers laugh considerably more than female listeners; e. male speakers laugh 7% less than female listeners; f. female speakers laugh 127% more than male listeners; and g. speakers usually laugh at the end of complete phrases (rather than in the middle), as a kind of nonverbal punctuation.
Neuro-notes I. Visual, auditory, tactile, and vestibular (but rarely
smell or taste) cues stimulate laughter's complex, reverberating chain of events
involving areas of the brain stem, hypothalamus, and frontal lobes, as well as centers of
the motor and cognitive cerebral cortex.
Neuro-notes II.
1. "Researchers may have found the location of [the] sense of humor in
the brain, according to their presentation at the 86th Scientific Assembly and
Annual Meeting of the Radiological Society of North America (RSNA) in Chicago,
Illinois. Humor appreciation appears to be based in the lower frontal lobes of
the brain, a location associated with social and emotional judgment and
planning, according to imaging research" (Flapan 2000). 2. "'As with
almost any behavior, we found that laughing at a joke involves several parts of
the brain,' said Dr. [Dean K.] Shibata [assistant professor of radiology at the
University of Rochester School of Medicine in New York]. 'Our [fMRI] imaging
results show that while the ventromedial frontal lobe is likely the center for
telling you what's funny, the accompanying laughter and feeling of mirth may be
triggered by connections to other areas of the brain [including the nucleus
accumbens; see PLEASURE CUE] which are involved in motor control [moving
the mouth] and positive emotions'" (Flapan 2000).
Neuro-notes III. Mirror neurons: As in human beings, the laughter of an individual chimpanzee can trigger imitative laughter in chimps nearby (source: Davila-Ross, Marina; Allcock, Bethan; Thomas, C.; and Kim Bard (2011). "Aping Expressions? Chimpanzees Produce Distinct Laugh Types When Responding to Laughter of Others," in Emotion, Vol. 11, No. 5, pp. 1113-20). In both species, imitative laughter is likely due to mirror neurons.
Neuro-notes IV. Mirror neurons: "Rapid facial mimicry (RFM) is an automatic response, in which individuals mimic others' expressions. RFM, only demonstrated in humans and apes, is grounded in the automatic perception-action coupling of sensorimotor information occurring in the mirror neuron system. In humans, RFM [as in laughing] seems to reflect the capacity of individuals to empathize with others. Here, we demonstrated that, during play, RFM is also present in a cercopithecoid species (Theropithecus gelada) [in the "play face," thought to be homologous with human laughter]." [Source: Mancini, Giada, Ferrari, Pier F., and Elisabetta Palagi (2013). "Rapid Facial Mimicry in Geladas." In Scientific Reports (Vol. 3, 28 March 2013; http://www.nature.com/srep/2013/130328/srep01527/full/srep01527.html).]
See also ISOPRAXISM.
Copyright 1998 - 2016 (David B. Givens/Center for Nonverbal Studies)
Photo of "Smile, Surfacing" (Fiesta Mexicana, Spokane, Washington, USA) by Doreen K. Givens (copyright 2007)